The
session to be discussed below was titled "Links and
Relations," which is absolutely correct as a description and
also intellectually inspiring because it at least raises the
question whether "links" are "relations" and, if so, of what
type. Or, to put it differently, exactly when and from what
state of exploration on a "link" is seen as a "relation."
This certainly does not apply to a first discovery of the
link anchor promising a relation; at this stage, it is in no
way certain whether that promise will hold. The four
contributions presented in this session can be seen under
this aspect of modeling of expectations, and inspiration can
be taken from the contribution at the beginning, which won
the "Newcomer Award:" "A Pragmatics of Links" by Susana
Pajares
Tosca (University of Madrid). She and Mark
Bernstein dealt more with this aspect of
micromodeling, while the two other contributions, by Licia
Calvi and Locke
Carter, discussed
macromodeling. Let us move on from the big things to small
ones.
Licia Calvis
contribution (Trinity College, Dublin) was not presented at
the congress, but should not be disregarded here for that
reason, for she deals with an important aspect of media
difference between a "book" and "hypertext" on the basis of
a short story by an English author available both as a book
and as internet HT. So, if the content is more or less the
same, how does the medium change experience with the text
(and hypertext, respectively) and, perhaps, even with the
content? In this case, the content is about a ride in a
London tube as presented by descriptions of the maximum
possible number of 253 passengers (7x36 plus the driver),
which adds up to one fate per page of the book version.
Calvi arrives at the finding that, of course, the
reading experience will be different as a function of the
medium, but the contents will not be affected by the medium.
In this case, however, this was due to the less than optimal
conversion into HT.
"When faced with the task of
constructing single-author, self-contained arguments in
hypertext environment, ... authors must overcome the
expectation of order." Thus, anyway (Proceedings, p. 85),
the usual expectation of a connection between a logical
chain of arguments and the disappearance of order in the HT
as a result of the design principle could be described (as
explained by Bolter or Landow). However, and this is the
gist of this contribution by Locke Carter (Texas Tech
University) about "Arguments in Hypertext," this connection
no longer has such a dramatic impact if one looks at more
recent approaches, for instance in the area of "informal
logic" (Toulmin, Perelman) or the "stasis theory"
(Fulkerson). In that case, it no longer mattered in what
sequence arguments appeared; what mattered was that they
appeared at all, as one was able to observe in arguments
exchanged in court (Proceedings, p. 88): "Since stasis
theory views the entire 'argument act' as one set of items
with little regard to order, hypertext argumentation may
profit from adapting the classical stases and developing new
ones."
What is the purpose of a
link anchor in communication? This question was examined by
Mark Bernstein, an untiring, always inspiring HT
propagandist with Eastgate Systems. His contribution
contains the argument with an ironic twist already in the
title: "More than legible: On links that readers don't want
to follow." However, it was the dual purpose of a "link" to
make readers follow the link and to branch logically from
the existing context. Exploiting this leeway had been the
main content of innovations in the past few years. In
Bernstein's view, a prototype of an input-sensitive dynamics
of a link anchor is an approach which Jim Rosenberg
presented at the last conference in Darmstadt with his
"intergrams:" layers of text superimposed to the point of
illegibility which, after a number of mouse clicks,
gradually became disentangled, thus revealing their
contents.
Susana Tosca argues
explicitly on the basis of contributions by Jim Rosenberg
(at the '96 conference in Washington), Mark Bernstein (at
the '98 conference in Pittsburgh) and by Wendy Morgan (at
last year's conference in Darmstadt) and, in this way,
indicates that this series of conferences indeed represents
a community of discourse. She is interested in shedding
light on those processes which occur in the microrange of
deciding about the conformity of expectations held
vis-à-vis branching offers: to click or not to click?
This largely parallels my own approach based on reception
psychology. It marks a level far below the large patterns on
which Bernstein worked, also the agglomerations of
(clicking) actemes studied by Rosenberg, and ranks even
above the assessments which Wendy Morgan made her topic.
Tosca argues: "... links force us to make meaning
before and after traveling them." Which means
that, when encountering a link the very first time, the
reader must form expectations about what the link may
produce, can then formulate the appropriate alternative (as
Tosca does in following a specific approach, the "relevance
theory"), and then see, after reading, whether these
expectations came true or not. This intensive search for
meaning and a context developing the interpretation, turn
links "poetic."
This two-step semantic
exploration of meaning could be the main point in the
experience of reading hypertext. This, at least, was the
message one could take home. On the other hand, it makes you
wonder why Tosca meanwhile discontinued her ambitious
reading experiment, announced in Darmstadt last year, with a
text by James Joyce. Also William Collin no longer
pursues his experiment of reading the "Cantos" by Ezra
Pound. In my view, both cases are indicative of the fact
that reading hypertext really is not easy.
0
- 1
- 2
- 3
>
4
- 5
- 6
- 7
- 8